Show/Hide Menu
Hide/Show Apps
Logout
Türkçe
Türkçe
Search
Search
Login
Login
OpenMETU
OpenMETU
About
About
Open Science Policy
Open Science Policy
Open Access Guideline
Open Access Guideline
Postgraduate Thesis Guideline
Postgraduate Thesis Guideline
Communities & Collections
Communities & Collections
Help
Help
Frequently Asked Questions
Frequently Asked Questions
Guides
Guides
Thesis submission
Thesis submission
MS without thesis term project submission
MS without thesis term project submission
Publication submission with DOI
Publication submission with DOI
Publication submission
Publication submission
Supporting Information
Supporting Information
General Information
General Information
Copyright, Embargo and License
Copyright, Embargo and License
Contact us
Contact us
How do Turkish speakers interpret pronouns in Transfer-of-Possession contexts?
Date
2018-10-19
Author
Özge, Duygu
Kesici, Alper
Köse, Engin
Metadata
Show full item record
Item Usage Stats
300
views
0
downloads
Cite This
How we identify the antecedent of an ambiguous pronoun has been a topic of interest in discourse anaphora studies. Crudely speaking, the most prominent entity is selected as the antecedent, but what determines the ranking of an entity as prominent is still an open question. Features such as givenness,[1, 2, 3] parallel roles,[4] or recency,[3] syntactic role,[5] thematic role, [6,7] verb type, [7] coherence relations, [3,8, 9] and referential form[3,10] have been nominated as the determinants of prominence in anaphora resolution. Other studies suggest that multiple factors interact during pronoun resolution.[10,11,12, 13] Also, a widely accepted generalization is that more reduced referential forms encode more prominent entities.[14] For Turkish, the intuition is that null pronouns are likely to refer to the subject and overt pronouns refer to the object.[15] However, recent experimental studies showed some verb types (e.g., stimulus-experiencer; e.g., frighten) in Turkish may not reflect this expectation due to their strong thematic biases. [16; c.f., 10,17] Similarly, for English, Transfer-of-Possession (ToP) verbs have been reported to show a goal-bias.[6] Yet, recent studies underlined the influence of coherence relations: the goal-bias is observed only in occasion (and) and result (so) relations but not in explanation relations.[9] For Japanese[17] and Korean,[18] on the other hand, null pronouns were source-biased regardless of the coherence relations. Apparently, language-specific factors are also at play. However, all of these studies come from sentence-completion studies where the coherence relations may be marked implicitly. What happens in comprehension when the coherence relations are overtly marked? We test how an ambiguous pronoun is interpreted in Turkish sentences with ToP verbs, and how different coherence markers (and, so, because) (manipulated within-subjects) and referential forms (null/overt) (manipulated between-subjects) influence interpretation. We conducted a rating study modeled after a previous study. [19] One-hundred-twelve participants read conjoined clauses with an ambiguous anaphor and a nonsense-verb, and they determine the antecedent of this nonsense action (see,1&2). If purely thematic factors dominate[6], we expect a goal-bias. If grammatical position determine the antecedent of a null pronoun as in Japanese[17] or Korean, [18] we expect more source-bias in null-pronoun condition regardless of the coherence marker. If coherence relations interact with the referential form as in English,[9] we expect a goal-bias in occasion and result conditions. We found a significant effect of coherence-marker and referential form, and a significant interaction between the two (see, Table & Figure). We observed a gradient source(subject)-bias changing with the coherence marker only in null-pronoun condition (source-bias: and>so>because). In this condition, there was a goal(object)-bias in occasion and result conditions and a source-bias in explanation condition. In the overt-pronoun condition, there was a goal-bias regardless of the coherence marker. Thus, even if reflecting different thematic biases, Turkish null-pronouns pattern with English pronouns (and not with Japanese/Korean nullpronouns) in reflecting a complex interplay between grammatical and pragmatic factors, but the overt-pronouns are under the heavy influence of grammatical factors. They are predominantly linked to the object.[15]
URI
https://wafl14.mit.edu/sites/default/files/WAFL14_paper_27.pdf
https://hdl.handle.net/11511/84260
Conference Name
Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics, (19 - 21 Ekim 2018)
Collections
Department of Foreign Language Education, Conference / Seminar
Suggestions
OpenMETU
Core
Analysis of the social meanings of the second person pronoun SEN'in Turkish
Hatipoğlu, Çiler (2008-04-01)
Bu çalışma, üniversite öğrencilerinin ikinci tekil şahıs adılı SEN’i ne zaman ve kime hitap ederken kullandıklarını incelemekte ve kullanılan ikinci şahıs adılının araştırmaya katılanlar için hangi toplumsal ve anlatımsal anlamları taşıdığını bulmaya çalışmaktadır. Çalışmada ayrıca, Brown ve Gilman’ın (1960) ortaya koydukları “Güç ve Dayanışma” modelinin Türkçe’de gözlenen SEN kullanım çeşitlerini ve bunların taşıdıkları anlamları açıklayıp açıklayamadığını da araştırmaktadır. Çalışmada kullanılan veriler a...
Do non-native languages have an effect on word order processing in first language Turkish?
Cedden, Gülay (2019-08-01)
Aims and Objectives/Purpose/Research Questions: Existing studies on sentence processing in bi-/multilinguals are typically centred on the first language (L1) influence on second language sentence processing. However, there is almost no evidence of influence in the other direction. The aim of this study is to find out whether being mono-, bi-, tri- or plurilingual has an effect on reading times (RTs) in the native language. To this end, Turkish native speakers' RTs are measured when processing Turkish canoni...
The Processing of ambiguous morphemes in Turkish
Ataman, Esra.; Kırkıcı, Bilal; Department of English Literature (2019)
Studies investigating the processing of linguistic ambiguity have to date mostly focused on lexical ambiguity. Morphemic ambiguity, on the other hand, has been less frequently studied in spite of its cross-linguistic prevalence. An intermediate level of representation (i.e. the lemma level) between form and meaning has been claimed to successfully account for the processing of ambiguous morphemes in English and Chinese. Moreover, meaning frequency has been found to affect the processing of these morphemes. ...
How does blogging enhance pre-service english language teachers' reflectivity in practicum?
Korkmazgil, Sibel; Gürbüz, Nurdan; Department of English Language Teaching (2009)
This was a case study which aimed to examine how blogging enhanced reflection among pre-service English language teachers in Practicum. 12 pre-service English language teachers enrolled in the undergraduate program of English Teaching Education in the Department of Foreign Language Education at Middle East Technical University participated in the study which comprised a 12-week time span in the 2007-2008 spring term. Data collection consisted of archival records of participants’ blog posts and comments, pre...
Appropriateness of a cognitive approach to Donald Davidson's meaning theory
Ağoğlu, Eser; Sayan, Erdinç; Department of Philosophy (2008)
The purpose of this study is to discuss the appropriateness of a cognitive approach to Donald Davidson's meaning theory. Davidson makes the bold proposal that a truth theory, modified for a natural language, may be treated as a meaning theory for that language. According to Davidson, a meaning theory is an empirical theory. Radical Interpretation is at the center of such an empirical inquiry which places restrictions on the truth theory to make it suitable as a meaning theory without appeal to semantic noti...
Citation Formats
IEEE
ACM
APA
CHICAGO
MLA
BibTeX
D. Özge, A. Kesici, and E. Köse, “How do Turkish speakers interpret pronouns in Transfer-of-Possession contexts?,” presented at the Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics, (19 - 21 Ekim 2018), Boston, Amerika Birleşik Devletleri, 2018, Accessed: 00, 2021. [Online]. Available: https://wafl14.mit.edu/sites/default/files/WAFL14_paper_27.pdf.